Journal Information
Vol. 82. Issue 5.
Pages 308-315 (1 May 2015)
Visits
6194
Vol. 82. Issue 5.
Pages 308-315 (1 May 2015)
Original Article
Full text access
Somatic complaints, emotional awareness and maladjustment in schoolchildren
Quejas somáticas, conciencia emocional e inadaptación en población escolar
Visits
6194
A. Ordóñeza,
Corresponding author
ana.ordonez@uv.es

Corresponding author.
, C. Magantob, R. Gonzáleza
a Departamento de Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamientos Psicológicos, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
b Departamento de Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamientos Psicológicos, Universidad del País Vasco, San Sebastián, Guipúzcoa, Spain
This item has received
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Tables (5)
Table 1. Prevalence of somatic complaints (from highest to lowest).
Table 2. Frequency of somatic complaints.
Table 3. Differences in means in the analysed variables by level of somatic complaints (ANOVA).
Table 4. Pearson's correlation between the different analysed variables (n=1134).
Table 5. Regression analysis of the variables evaluated concerning somatic complaints.
Show moreShow less
Abstract
Introduction

Somatic complaints are common in childhood. Research has shown their relationship with emotional awareness and maladjustment. The study had three objectives: 1) to analyse the prevalence of somatic complaints; 2) to explore the relationships between the variables evaluated: somatic complaints, differentiating emotions, verbal sharing of emotions, not hiding emotions, body awareness, attending to others’ emotions, analysis of emotions, and personal, social, family, and school maladjustments; and 3) to identify predictors of somatic complaints.

Patients and methods

The study included a total of 1134 randomly selected schoolchildren of both sexes between 10 and 12 years old (M=10.99; SD=0.88). The Somatic Complaint List, Emotional Awareness Questionnaire, and Self-Reported Multifactor Test of Childhood Adaptation were used to gather information.

Results

The results showed that the prevalence of somatic complaints was 90.2%, with fatigue, headache and stomachache being the most frequent. Dizziness and headache were more common in girls, and the frequency of complaints decreases with age. Somatic complaints are negatively related to emotional awareness, and positively related to maladjustment. The variables that contribute the most to the prediction of somatic complaints are personal maladjustment (25.1%) and differentiating emotions (2.5%).

Conclusions

The study shows that personal maladjustment is the best predictor of somatic complaints; the more emotional awareness and better adapted the child, the fewer somatic complaints they lodge. Childhood is a stage with significant physical discomfort.

Keywords:
Somatic complaints
Emotional intelligence
Social adjustment
Psychological adjustment
School age population
Resumen
Introducción

Las quejas somáticas son frecuentes en la infancia. La investigación ha puesto de manifiesto su relación con la conciencia emocional y la inadaptación. El estudio tuvo 3 objetivos: 1) analizar la prevalencia de las quejas somáticas; 2) explorar las relaciones entre las variables evaluadas: quejas somáticas, diferenciar emociones, comunicar verbalmente emociones, no esconder emociones, conciencia corporal, atender emociones de otros, análisis de las propias emociones, inadaptación personal, inadaptación social, inadaptación familiar e inadaptación escolar, y 3) identificar variables predictoras de las quejas somáticas.

Pacientes y métodos

Los participantes, seleccionados aleatoriamente, fueron 1.134 escolares de 10–12 años (media±desviación típica=10,99±0,88), de ambos sexos. Se aplicaron el Listado de quejas somáticas, cuestionario de conciencia emocional y el Test autoevaluativo multifactorial de adaptación infantil.

Resultados

Los resultados indicaron que la prevalencia de quejas somáticas es del 90,2%, siendo el cansancio, el dolor de cabeza y el dolor de estómago los más frecuentes. Los mareos y el dolor de cabeza son superiores en niñas y las quejas disminuyen con la edad. Las quejas somáticas se relacionan negativamente con la conciencia emocional y positivamente con la inadaptación. Las variables que más contribuyen en la predicción de las quejas somáticas son la inadaptación personal (25.1%) y diferenciar emociones (2.5%).

Conclusiones

El trabajo permite concluir que la inadaptación personal es la mejor predictora de las quejas somáticas, así a mayor conciencia emocional y mejor adaptación, menor es el número de quejas somáticas presentadas, siendo la infancia una etapa evolutiva con importante malestar físico.

Palabras clave:
Quejas somáticas
Inteligencia emocional
Adaptación social
Adaptación psicológica
Población infantil
Full Text
Introduction

Somatic complaints (SCs) are body-related perceptions with subjectively experienced unpleasant qualities.1,2 Twenty-five percent of children seen at the paediatrician's office have physical symptoms that are better explained as psychosomatic manifestations rather than medical conditions.3 Epidemiology studies show an increase in the prevalence of SCs, with 5–30% of children 8–16 years of age affected by them.3,6,7 Some studies identify stomachache as the most common SC, with a prevalence that ranges between 8% and 25% of schoolchildren,8 while others reported fatigue (12.1%)1 or headache (58.5%) as the most prevalent.9,10 When we consider sex and age, the prevalence of SCs is higher in girls than in boys,1,8,10,11 and in early childhood and early adolescence.12

In the absence of identifiable organic causes, there is growing interest in the processes at play in psychosocial factors.2,5,10–14 This study analysed two possible factors: emotional functioning as manifested by emotion awareness, and the maladjustment of the child in various settings.

The association between emotional functioning and SCs2,15,16 suggests that alexithymia, a limited ability to identify and express emotions, is the main characteristic of individuals with SCs.17,18 There is evidence of this association in the paediatric population,13,19 as it has been observed that an inability to differentiate emotions and a strong bodily awareness in the course of experiencing emotion are predictors for SCs.13,20 Van der Veek et al2 noted that children with poor emotional awareness have difficulty recognising the emotions they experience and coping effectively with them. The use of maladaptive coping strategies leads to an increase in somatisation in response to the unresolved emotional conflict.21

Recurrent SCs can lead to psychosocial impairment and have a negative impact on the child's family life, ability to concentrate, cognitive development and academic achievement.4,9,16,21,22 Furthermore, children with SCs have poorer school attendance, participate less in extracurricular activities, and have poorer interpersonal relationships.4,5,9,10,22 Vila et al22 observed that 47% of children with SCs had difficulty concentrating, 30% missed school, and 24% spent time with friends less frequently. The presence of SCs as a result of personal maladjustment and social problems has also been studied. In this regard, conflict with peers or in the family environment leads to negative moods that in turn increase the likelihood of SCs.6,15,23,24

In conclusion, SCs are common in childhood, yet there is little research on the subject.25 There is evidence that the aetiology of SCs involves psychosocial factors in addition to medical factors. This underscores the importance of knowing the variables that may influence SCs in order to develop effective interventions.

In this context, we established three objectives: (1) to analyse the prevalence of SCs; (2) to explore the relationship between the variables under study; and (3) to identify predictor for SCs. Based on the findings of previous studies, we formulated the following hypotheses: (1) the prevalence of SCs is between 5% and 30%, and the most prevalent complaints are headache and stomachache; (2) SCs correlates negatively to emotional awareness variables and positively to maladjustment variables; and (3) the variables that can predict SCs are differentiating emotions, verbal sharing of emotions, bodily awareness and social maladjustment.

Patients and methodsParticipants

The participants were 1134 children between 10 and 12 years of age (mean±SD=10.99±0.80), with a homogeneous sex and age distribution (53.4% female; 46.6% male; 371 age 10 years; 401 age 11 years; 362 age 12 years), enrolled in 12 public and charter schools in the autonomous community of Valencia (Spain). We used cluster sampling, taking into account the cities and their number of inhabitants, the type of institution (public or charter school), the number of students in each school, and the age groups.

Measurement instruments

We evaluated SCs by means of the Somatic Complaint List (SCL).26 This instrument assesses how frequently the individual has experienced pain in the preceding 4 weeks. It consists of 11 items with three answer choices (1=never; 2=sometimes; 3=often) that add to an overall score. Of these items in the scale, one refers to overall pain and eight to specific SCs: tiredness, stomachache, weakness, dizziness, headache, nausea, feeling faint, and chest pain. Two are contrasting items and refer to wellness: “I feel healthy” and “I feel well”. This questionnaire has shown good reliability (α=.81) and good psychometric properties (NFI=.95; CFI=.94; IFI=.94; RMSEA=.07) (P<.001) in previous studies.27

Emotional awareness was measured by means of the Emotion Awareness Questionnaire (EAQ),13 which has 30 items with 3 answer choices (1=not true; 2=sometimes true; 3=true). It assesses 6 dimensions: differentiating emotions (ability to identify, differentiate and understand the cause of emotions, 7 items); verbal sharing of emotions (sharing own feelings with others, 3 items); not hiding emotions (open and appropriate expression of emotions, 5 items); bodily awareness (ability to make the connection between physical sensations and emotions, 5 items); attending to others’ emotions (an interest in listening to and/or perceiving others’ emotions, 5 items); and analysing emotions (an interest in perceiving and understanding own emotions, 5 items). The higher the score in a given dimension, the higher the ability is supposed to be in that area. The factors have shown acceptable reliability (between α=0.63 and α=0.68) and the factor structure has been confirmed for the Spanish version (maximum likelihood method): NFI=0.92; CFI=0.91; IFI=0.91; RMSEA=0.03 (P<.001)27.

Maladjustment was assessed by means of the Test autoevaluativo multifactorial de adaptación infantil (Multifactor Self-Assessment Test of Child Adjustment [TAMAI]).28 It evaluates maladjustment and consists of 172 dichotomous items (0=no, 1=yes). In every dimension, higher scores correspond to higher levels of maladjustment. We used four of its scales: personal (distorted view of oneself and difficulty facing reality, range 0–35), social (lack of control, respect and consideration towards others, reduced social interaction and mistrust, range 0–35), family (dissatisfaction with the family environment and the relationship with parents, range 0–5) and school (low achievement, disruptive behaviours and negative attitude, range 0–30). The reliability of the used scales was adequate (α=0.82, α=0.78, α=0.69 and α=0.88, respectively), reaching a reliability of α=0.89 in the sample of the authors.28

These self-assessment tools were designed to be used from 8 years of age, once the child is capable of reporting his or her feelings and experiences.29

Study design and protocol

The study had a descriptive cross-sectional design. We contacted the schools to explain the objectives of the study, seek their collaboration, and request their consent. We sent a letter to parents explaining the study and requesting their consent for participation.

We performed the statistical analysis of the data with the SPSS software (version 21). We calculated descriptive statistics for the variables under study and Pearson's bivariate correlations to study the association between the variables, performed Student's t tests and multivariate analyses of variance to study the differences between groups, and finally did stepwise hierarchical regression analyses, testing the necessary assumptions.30

Ethical considerations

This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Conselleria d’Educació de la Comunitat Valenciana (Education Department of the Autonomous Community of Valencia), adhering to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the current law. Self-administered questionnaires were given out by teachers to entire classes of students during school hours in two sessions of 45min each.

ResultsPrevalence

The most prevalent complaint was tiredness, reported by 81.3% of the assessed children; followed by headache (45.7%) and stomach ache (43.7%), while feeling faint was the least prevalent complaint (11.7%) (Table 1).

Table 1.

Prevalence of somatic complaints (from highest to lowest).

Somatic complaints  Prevalence 
1. I feel tired  81.3% 
2. I have a headache  45.7% 
3. I have a stomachache  43.7% 
4. I feel weak  30.3% 
5. I feel dizzy  30.2% 
6. My chest hurts  22.9% 
7. I feel nauseated  22.8% 
8. I feel like I am going to faint  11.7% 

When we considered the number of reported SCs, we found that only 9.8% of the assessed children reported none; 40% of children reported one or two; and 12.9% six or more complaints (Table 2). For the purpose of this analysis, a complaint existed if the participant answered “sometimes” or “often” for it.

Table 2.

Frequency of somatic complaints.

Somatic complaints  F  Cumulative % 
No complaints  111  9.8  9.8 
1 complaint  253  22.3  32.1 
2 complaints  212  18.7  50.8 
3 complaints  165  14.6  65.3 
4 complaints  127  11.2  76.5 
5 complaints  120  10.6  87.1 
6 complaints  66  5.8  92.9 
7 complaints  42  3.7  96.6 
8 complaints  38  3.4  100 
Total  1134  100  100 
Analysis of sex- and age-related differences

Our results showed statistically significant differences between the sexes, with girls reporting a higher prevalence of SCs such as “I feel weak” (P.004) and “I have a headache” (P.006). They also scored higher in EAQ subscales like bodily awareness (P.001), attending to others’ emotions (P.001) and analyses of emotions (P.001). Boys scored higher in the TAMAI scale for social maladjustment (P.001) and school maladjustment (P.001).

We also found statistically significant differences by age in some SCs: “I feel weak” (P.003), “I feel dizzy” (P.008) and “I feel nauseated” (P.032). There were also differences in the differentiating emotions (P.001) and bodily awareness (P.009) variables of the EAQ, and in the personal maladjustment (P.001) and family maladjustment (P.001) of the TAMAI, suggesting that the older the child, the lower the level of SCs correlated to every variable, except for differentiating emotions. The effect size was small in all instances.

Emotional awareness and maladjustment: differences by somatic complaint

To analyse the differences in emotional awareness and maladjustment in relation to somatic complaints, we divided the participants into groups by the score obtained in the SCL, stratified by tertiles. The results of the analysis showed statistically significant differences in every analysed variable except for analyses of emotions (Table 3), as children with low levels of SCs had higher scores in differentiating emotions (P.001), verbal sharing of emotions (P.001), not hiding emotions (P.001), attending to others’ emotions (P.001) and analyses of own emotions (P.035). Meanwhile, they scored lower in bodily awareness (P.001) and in personal (P.001), social (P.001), family (P.001) and school maladjustment (P.001). Although post hoc Tukey's tests confirmed the differences between the three groups, the effect size was low.

Table 3.

Differences in means in the analysed variables by level of somatic complaints (ANOVA).

Variables  Level of somatic complaintsF  P 
  Low n=437M±SD  Medium n=341M±SD  High n=356M±SD     
EAQ
Differentiating emotions  2.45±0.42  2.36±0.40  2.19±0.40  37.241  .001 
Verbal sharing of emotions  2.22±0.54  2.05±0.56  1.90±0.54  33.150  .001 
Not hiding emotions  2.13±0.54  2.05±0.56  1.90±0.54  8.147  .001 
Bodily awareness  2.02±0.55  2.15±0.51  2.28±0.45  25.578  .001 
Attending to others’ emotions  2.64±0.39  2.60±0.37  2.53±0.41  7.413  .001 
Analyses of emotions  2.40±0.44  2.38±0.42  2.32±0.41  3.364  .035 
TAMAI
Personal maladjustment  7.29±4.27  9.48±4.76  12.88±5.7  125.037  .001 
Social maladjustment  6.38±4.12  7.45±3.91  8.95±5.14  33.059  .001 
Family maladjustment  0.42±0.91  0.48±0.97  0.84±1.22  17.889  .001 
School maladjustment  6.65±5.02  7.57±5.33  9.42±6.40  24.271  .001 

M: mean; SD: standard deviation.

Analysis of correlation

The correlation analyses (Table 4) showed inverse correlations between SCs and the variables differentiating emotions (r=−0.305; P.001), verbal sharing of emotions (r=−0.257; P.001) and attending to others’ emotions (r=−0.131; P.001), and positive correlations between SCs and bodily awareness (r=0.229; P.001), personal (r=0.495; P.001), social (r=0.285; P.001), family (r=0.213; P.001) and school maladjustment (r=0.223; P.001).

Table 4.

Pearson's correlation between the different analysed variables (n=1134).

  SCL  EAQTAMAI
  10 
EAQ
−0.305***  –                 
−0.257***  0.316***  –               
−0.163***  0.272***  0.331***  –             
0.229***  −0.287***  −0.192***  −0.159***  –           
−0.131***  0.029  0.148***  0.124***  0.123***  –         
−0.058  −0.195***  0.040  −0.051  0.246***  0.404***  –       
TAMAI
0.495***  −0.308***  −0.322***  −0.251***  0.183***  −0.216***  −0.118***  –     
0.285***  −0.197***  −0.261***  −0.205***  0.040  −0.295***  −0.185***  0.581***  –   
10  0.213***  −0.167***  −0.122***  −0.075*  −0.002  −0.086**  −0.071*  0.312***  0.241***  – 
11  0.223***  −0.151***  −0.157***  −0.139***  −0.002  −0.246***  −0.226***  0.445***  0.624***  0.220*** 

1: somatic complaints; 2: differentiating emotions; 3: verbal sharing of emotions; 4: not hiding emotions; 5: bodily awareness; 6: attending to others’ emotions; 7: analyses of emotions; 8: personal maladjustment; 9: social maladjustment; 10: family maladjustment; 11: school maladjustment.

*

P<.05.

**

P<.01.

***

P<.001.

Predictive analyses for somatic complaints

The linear regression analysis with SCs as the criterion variable and emotional awareness and maladjustment as predictors (Table 5) showed that six of the predictor variables were statistically significant: personal maladjustment (β=0.394; ΔR2=0.251), differentiating emotions (β=−0.133; ΔR2=0.026), bodily awareness (β=0.122; ΔR2=0.011), analyses of own emotions (β=−0.058; ΔR2=0.004), verbal sharing of emotions (β=−0.059; ΔR2=0.003) and family maladjustment (β=0.057; ΔR2=0.003). The beta standardised regression coefficients suggest that the influence of these variables on SCs in small, except for personal maladjustment. This group of variables explains 29.4% of the SC variance.

Table 5.

Regression analysis of the variables evaluated concerning somatic complaints.

  R  R2  ΔR2  Standard error  B  Standard error  Constant  β  t  P 
Personal maladjustment  0.501  0.251  0.131  3.05  0.254  0.019  12.150  0.394  13.447  .001 
Differentiating emotions  0.527  0.277  0.094  3.00  −1.104  0.239  15.756  −0.133  −4.612  .001 
Bodily awareness  0.537  0.288  0.017  2.98  0.819  0.185  13.713  0.122  4.419  .001 
Analyses of own emotions  0.540  0.292  0.016  2.97  −0.470  0.222  15.072  −0.058  −2.115  .035 
Verbal sharing of emotions  0.543  0.295  0.031  2.97  −0.369  0.174  15.661  −0.059  −2.119  .034 
Family maladjustment  0.545  0.298  0.003  2.96  0.189  0.090  15.426  0.057  2.097  .036 
Discussion

Our objectives were to analyse the prevalence of SCs, explore their relationship with emotional awareness and maladjustment, and identify predictor variables. The results showed differences between sexes and age groups, but these variables were not included in subsequent analyses due to their small effect size. The results we obtained were not consistent with the existing literature,9,10 as girls only scored higher than boys in having a headache and feeling nauseated. When it came to age, we observed an inverse correlation with SCs, rather than the increase in frequency described in the previous literature,1,9,12 and we also did not find a greater prevalence of headache in older children.

Based on our initial hypotheses, we expected the prevalence of SCs to be between 5% and 30%.3,6 Only 9.8% of participants reported being free from SCs, which means that 90.2% had experienced some type of pain in the past few weeks. And while 22.3% of participants reported a single SC, this was not the usual pattern, as 67.9% of participants reported having two or more. In this sense, our results showed a greater prevalence of SCs in childhood,4,5 and were alarming due to the high rates reported and the sample not being a clinical but a community sample. The data showed similar prevalences of headache and stomachache (45.7% and 43.7%, respectively), which were the most prevalent SCs in the literature we reviewed.8,9 However, the most prevalent SC was tiredness (81.3%), similar to the fatigue reported by Barkmann et al.1 When we consider the pace of life in current society, it makes sense for children to feel tired, as it is difficult to balance work and family life. The daily routine includes the school day and coordinating the parents’ work schedules with homework and extracurricular activities (foreign language study, music, sports). This situation results in longer days, delayed bedtimes, and a reduction in the hours of sleep. The high prevalence we observed may also be due to characteristics of our sample or to the self-reporting nature of the instruments used in our assessment. However, the fact that these self-reporting tools have been used in the past and that the observed results are consistent with the literature supports this hypothesis.

Our second hypothesis stated that SCs would be negatively correlated with emotional awareness variables and positively correlated to maladjustment variables. On this point, as expected, SCs were negatively correlated to the differentiating emotions, verbal sharing of emotions, not hiding emotions, and attending to others’ emotional variables.2,15,21 We would like to underscore the positive correlation between SCs and bodily awareness. Similar to previous studies that used the EAQ2,21 and with the purpose of making a homogeneous interpretation of the analyses of every aspect of emotional awareness, we computed the score of the bodily awareness subscale without reversing the items. The results seem confusing, as being aware of the physical sensations that accompany emotions should be a good thing, but it is associated to SCs. However, they are similar to results obtained in previous studies,13,14 in which children that were more aware of the relationship between bodily sensations and emotions reported more somatic symptoms. One possible explanation is that these children are very aware of their sensations and are therefore more nervous, manifesting anxiety through SCs. On the other hand, the correlation between SCs and personal, social, family and school maladjustment was positive.4–6,9,10,16,21 We ought to note that the positive correlation between SCs and personal maladjustment was the highest we found in our results. This is relevant given how little evidence is available on the association of these variables, although there are studies that analyse the association of SCs with maladjustment and social, school, and family problems.4,6,9,16,23,24 Previous studies may have underestimated the importance of intrapersonal variables in favour of interpersonal variables due to the difficulties inherent in evaluating the former.

Our third hypothesis stated that aspects of emotion awareness associated with alexithymia would contribute most to explaining SCs 2,13 along with social maladjustment.4,16,23 Yet, the variable that explained the greatest amount of variance was personal maladjustment; while the other variables contributed less to it. As we noted above, few studies have analysed the association between SCs and personal maladjustment, although SCs have been associated with variables like self-esteem, quality of life, wellbeing, mood, and adjustment.2,4–6,9,15 The personal maladjustment scale of the TAMAI refers to the adjustment and balance of the individual with him or herself; it is a complex construct, but it can be used to observe the relevance of the child's inner life in his or her physical wellbeing. Considering our results, the study of personal aspects is important to understand what is happening to the child, how it is affecting him or her, and the ensuing consequences. From an early age, children have personal feelings and thoughts about what is happening in their environment, and it is important that we understand how they function at a personal level and how they face everyday conflicts. Thus, the more effective their coping mechanisms, the better their adjustment is, and the lesser their health and social problems.

One of the main limitations of our study is the exclusive use of self-reporting tools, although from 8 years of age children start having direct knowledge of their thoughts and feelings, and their answers can be reliable.29 It is also worth noting that the effect size was small in every case; however, we did observe significant differences, so we must consider what these results are pointing at, and explore it more thoroughly in future research. Lastly, it would be interesting to include different informants, like parents, paediatricians and teachers, to gain a broader understanding of the issue and its scope.

To conclude, the prevalence of SCs in children 10–12 years of age (community sample) was high, as 90.2% of participants reported having at least one SC. Furthermore, emotional awareness was important in explaining SCs, although the variable that contributed most to the presence of SCs was personal maladjustment. Thus, focusing exclusively on the association between emotional awareness and SCs is an oversimplified approach to the issue, as SCs are not only the result of poor emotional regulation through the day, but a physical response of the body to personal problems and conflicts.

Our study is relevant in that it contributes a new perspective to the understanding of SCs. Childhood is a developmental stage in which there is considerable physical pain requiring more attention.10,27 Collective interventions implemented in schools27 for the development of emotional skills and coping strategies may contribute to wellbeing, reducing the prevalence of SCs, the use of primary care healthcare services, and parental absences from work.

Funding

This study has been funded by:

  • The Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (Ministry of Science and Innovation, project PSI2010-18742).

  • The Consellería d’Educació de la Generalitat Valenciana (Department of Education of the Autonomous Community of Valencia, PROMETEO/2011/009).

  • The Universitat de València (Atracció de Talent fellowship for predoctoral researchers).

  • Departamento de Educación, Política Lingüística y Cultura (Department of Education, Political Linguistics and Culture) of the Autonomous Community of the Basque Country (IT638-13).

  • The Unidad de Formación e Investigación (Department of Training and Investigation) of the Universidad del País Vasco (UFIPSIXXI-11/04).

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References
[1]
C. Barkmann, E. Braehler, M. Schulte Markwort, A. Richterich.
Chronic somatic complaints in adolescents: prevalence, predictive validity of the parent reports, and associations with social class, health status, and psychosocial distress.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 46 (2011), pp. 1003-1011
[2]
S. Van der Veek, R. Nobel, H. Derkx.
The relationship between emotion awareness and somatic complaints in children and adolescents: investigating the mediating role of anxiety and depression.
Psychol Health, 0 (2012), pp. 1-16
[3]
C. López, M.V. Alcántara, V. Fernández, M. Castro, J.A. López.
Características y prevalencia de los problemas de ansiedad, depresión y quejas somáticas en una muestra clínica infantil de 8 a 12 años, mediante el CBCL.
An Psicol, 26 (2010), pp. 325-354
[4]
M. Saps, R. Seshadri, M. Sztainberg, G. Shaffer, B. Marshall, C. Di Lorenzo.
A prospective school-based study of abdominal pain and other common somatic complaints in children.
J Pediatr, 32 (2009), pp. 2-9
[5]
K. Weiss, A. Hahn, D. Wallace, B. Biggs, B. Bruce, T. Harrison.
Acceptance of pain: associations with depression, catastrophizing, and functional disability among children and adolescents in an interdisciplinary chronic pain rehabilitation program.
J Pediatr Psychol, 38 (2013), pp. 756-765
[6]
G. Gini, G. Carli, T. Pozzoli.
Social support, peer victimization, and somatic complaints: a meditational analysis.
J Paediatr Child Health, 45 (2009), pp. 358-363
[7]
S. Petersen, C. Brulin, E. Bergström.
Recurrent pain symptoms in young school children are often multiple.
[8]
L. Duffon, M. Dunn, B. Compas.
Anxiety and somatic complaints in children with recurrent abdominal pain and anxiety disorders.
J Pediatr Psychol, 34 (2009), pp. 176-186
[9]
T. Zolog, M.C. Jane Ballabriga, A. Bonillo-Martin, J. Canals-Sans, C. Hernández-Martínez, K. Tomero-Acosta, et al.
Somatic complaints and symptoms of anxiety and depression in a school-based sample of preadolescents and early adolescents. Funtional impairment and implications for treatment.
J Cogn Behav Psychother, 11 (2011), pp. 191-208
[10]
R. Serra, M. Claustre-Jane, A. Bonillo.
Síntomas somáticos funcionales en una muestra española: psicopatología y estilos educativos.
An Pediatr (Barc), 79 (2013), pp. 101-107
[11]
M. Wangby.
Somatic complaints in girls: a longitudinal perspective.
Cogn Behav Ther, 29 (2000), pp. 22-36
[12]
F. Jellesma, C. Rieffe, M. Meerum-Terwogt, C.M. Kneepkens.
Somatic complaints and health care use in children: mood, emotion awareness and sense of coherence.
Soc Sci Med, 63 (2006), pp. 2640-2648
[13]
C. Rieffe, P. Oosterveld, A.C. Miers, M. Meerum-Terwogt, V. Ly.
Emotion awareness and internalising symptoms in children and adolescents; the emotion awareness questionnaire revised.
Pers Individ Differ, 45 (2008), pp. 756-761
[14]
E. Vernberg, T. Nelson, P. Fonagy, S. Twemlow.
Victimization, aggression, and visits to the school nurse for somatic complaints, illnesses, and physical injuries.
Pediatrics, 127 (2011), pp. 842-848
[15]
F.C. Jellesma, C. Rieffe, M. Meerum Terwogt.
Do I feel sadness, fear or both? Comparing self-reported alexithymia and emotional task-performance in children with many or few somatic complaints.
Psychol Health, 24 (2009), pp. 881-893
[16]
F.C. Jellesma, C. Rieffe, M. Meerum-Terwogt.
My peers, my friend, and I: peer interactions and somatic complaints in boys and girls.
Soc Sci Med, 66 (2008), pp. 2195-2205
[17]
P.E. Sifneos.
The prevalence of alexithymic characteristics in psychosomatic patients.
Psychother Psychosom, 22 (1972), pp. 255-262
[18]
P.E. Sifneos.
Alexithymia: past and present.
Am J Psychiatry, 153 (1996), pp. 137-142
[19]
C. Rieffe, M. de Rooij.
The longitudinal relationship between emotion awareness and internalizing symptoms during late childhood.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 21 (2012), pp. 349-356
[20]
C. Rieffe, L. Villanueva, J.E. Adrián, A.B. Górriz.
Quejas somáticas, estados de ánimo y conciencia emocional en adolescentes.
Psicothema, 3 (2009), pp. 459-464
[21]
S. Van der Veek, H. Derkx, E. de Haan, M. Benninga, F. Boer.
Emotion awareness and coping in children with functional abdominal pain: a controlled study.
Soc Sci Med, 74 (2012), pp. 112-119
[22]
M. Vila, T. Kramer, N. Hickey, M. Dattani, H. Jefferis, M. Singh, et al.
Assessment of somatic symptoms in British secondary school children using the Children's Somatization Inventory (CSI).
J Pediatr Psychol, 34 (2009), pp. 989-998
[23]
C. Vilsaint, S. Aiyer, M. Wilson, D. Shaw, T. Dishion.
The ecology of early childhood risk: a canonical correlation analysis of children's adjustment, family, and community context in a high-risk sample.
J Prim Prev, 34 (2013), pp. 261-277
[24]
M. Orgilés, M.D. Samper.
El impacto del divorcio en la calidad de vida de los niños de 8 a 12 años en la provincial de Alicante.
Gac Sanit, 25 (2011), pp. 490-494
[25]
C. Colomer-Revuelta, J. Colomer-Revuelta, R. Mercer, R. Peiró-Pérez, L. Rajmil.
La salud en la infancia.
Gac Sanit, 18 (2004), pp. 39-46
[26]
F.C. Jellesma, C. Rieffe, M. Meerum-Terwogt.
The somatic complaint list: validation of a self-report questionnaire assessing somatic complaints in children.
J Psychosom Res, 63 (2007), pp. 399-401
[27]
A.B. Górriz, V.J. Prado-Gascó, L. Villanueva, A. Ordóñez, R. González.
The MOOD Questionnaire: adaptation and validation of the Spanish version.
Psicothema, 25 (2013), pp. 252-257
[28]
P. Hernández-Guanir.
Test autoevaluativo multifactorial de adaptación infantil (TAMAI).
TEA ediciones, (2009), pp. 86
[29]
A. Waters, J. Henry, K. Mogg, B. Bradley, D. Pine.
Attentional bias towards angry faces in childhood anxiety disorders.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry, 41 (2010), pp. 158-164
[30]
A. Field.
Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics.
SAGE Publications Ltd., (2013),

Please cite this article as: Ordóñez A, Maganto C, González R. Quejas somáticas, conciencia emocional e inadaptación en población escolar. An Pediatr (Barc). 2015;82:308–315.

Copyright © 2014. Asociación Española de Pediatría
Download PDF
Idiomas
Anales de Pediatría (English Edition)
Article options
Tools
es en

¿Es usted profesional sanitario apto para prescribir o dispensar medicamentos?

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?